Date: Mon, 08 Apr 1996 16:27:20 -0600 Subject: TREE article, part 2 of 3 Food sharing Food sharing is far more common among humans than among other primates. Among modern hunters, sharing varies with resource characteristics: large, unpredictably acquired packages are shared more widely than smaller items taken more predictably. Most analysts see this as risk-reduction: successful hunters give some of their catch to the unsuccessful in anticipation of a time when their respective fortunes are reversed.1,7,15-16 The free-rider problem lurking here15,17 is veiled by ethnographers' use of the term "reciprocity" to describe these transfers, absent any demonstration of the back and forth exchanges of food between pairs of individuals required for Trivers' reciprocal altruism. In well-described cases, some individuals consistently get shares even when they have not given while others continue to provide shares even if they are not repaid.17-18 Blurton Jones19 appealed to the economics of resource defense in suggesting that this pattern might be better described as "tolerated theft." If foragers take resources unpredictably, so that their successes are unsynchronized, in packages large enough that marginal benefits diminish across portions for any consumer, then those who have consumed less will value additional portions more. If the cost of contesting a portion is commensurate with its value to each contestant, then those who have less will take more. Acquirers have no "property rights" in the food they capture. If women forage to feed themselves and their families, the small defendable fraction of game animals plus the higher day-to-day probability of failure make hunting the inferior strategy for them.20 Why do men hunt? The economics of defense create a potential social benefit to foragers who acquire resources too expensive for anyone to monopolize. Since non-acquirers can expect to benefit, they have a direct interest in monitoring and exploiting the success of acquirers targeting such items. A forager attracts favorable attention by taking resources that many can expect to consume.18 This is an alternative to the long standing hypothesis that men hunt to provision their wives and offspring. Men's foraging may often be mating rather than parenting effort.20 Because of the marked differences in the defendability of various resource types foraging can serve different goals: either feeding one's "own" or attracting positive attention from potential allies and mates. Those seeking the former should pursue defendable resources, but favor companions who target items that are widely shared. Where men have more to gain >from allies and additional mates than from contributing to the welfare of wife and offspring, they should pursue widely shared foods that earn them favorable social attention. When additional mates are scarce, mens's foraging patterns may be shaped by mate guarding.21 Children's foraging and maternal tradeoffs The children of foragers can be surprisingly adept at food procurement.22 Mothers evidently adjust their own foraging tactics to take advantage of this, playing off their children's capabilites against their own in ways that maximize the rates they earn collectively.23 Tactics adopted in any particular situation vary greatly depending on both the array of resources available and age-related differences in procurement capabilities.24-25 Among non-human primates, juveniles feed themselves; thus mothers can only exploit resources their youngest weanling can acquire at a life sustaining rate. Because human mothers supply food to their offspring, they can rely on a wider array of resources, including those that young juveniles cannot manage. This practice expands the range of habitats that people, or food sharing hominids generally, can occupy. It also has a cost: the more dependent children are on mother's foraging, the larger the deficit they face when her attention is diverted to a newborn. Such costs can be offset by the efforts of an adult helper. Husband/father has long been cast in this role, establishing the nuclear family as unit of common economic and reproductive interest. But, as reviewed above, men seek goals that are often inconsistent with provisioning offspring. Grandmother is a more promising source of help for the weaned children of a nursing mother. The argument that nominates grandmothers as essential helpers draws evolutionary links among food sharing, the use of resources juveniles cannot handle adequately for themselves, and the long post-menopausal lifespans that distinguish human life histories >from those of other primates.20 Human life histories Humans differ from other primates in the timing and rate of life-course events. Not only do we have long post-menopausal lifespans, but we also mature later and have generally higher levels of fertility than chimpanzees. Life history theory provides the conceptual tools to investigate the evolution of these differences, as well as the variation in patterns of development and fertility among modern humans.26 Hunter-gatherer birth spacing was among the first topics to be addressed by human behavioral ecologists, and continuing work indicates the importance of tradeoffs in parental investment in explaining variation in patterns of fertility.22 The small but growing number of forager demographies shows wide variation in completed family sizes. Fertilities are lower where the character and distribution of resources is associated with age and sex specific foraging patterns that make children cost their mothers more.27-30 The proposition noted above, that mother-child food sharing depends on grandmothers' involvement, elaborates the hypothesis offered by G. C. Williams to explain the puzzle of post-menopausal lifespans. Since mutations acting after the age of last reproduction are usually assumed to have no effect on lifetime fitness, there should be no "post reproductive" life. Williams suggested that when extended maternal care was crucial to offspring success, continued child bearing by aging mothers might give lower marginal fitness gains than re-allocation of that parental investment to children already born. The first quantitative tests of this hypotheis using demographic data on Ache foragers do not support it.28 Alternative modeling31-32 and further assessment of grandmothering patterns are both in order. Hill and Hurtado's28 descriptions of Ache fertility and mortality patterns and comparisons with other populations illustrate the analytical leverage provided by Charnov's33 theory of "invariants" to explain life history evolution. A few variables in this general framework for explaining mammalian life histories capture enough empirical variation to account for age at maturity among the Ache and differences between the Ache and other hunter-gatherers. Since the theory links adult mortalities, growth rates, and reproductive rates, it offers a new way to measure differences between the life history patterns of humans and other living primates and pose hypotheses about hominid lifehistories that may be tested in the fossil record.26 --- from list marxism2-AT-lists.village.virginia.edu ---
Display software: ArchTracker © Malgosia Askanas, 2000-2005